Monkeys have a sense of morality, say scientists

  • Wanna Join? New users you can now register lightning fast using your Facebook or Twitter accounts.

ThaG

Sicc OG
Jun 30, 2005
9,597
1,687
113
#1
http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/news/environment/article5733638.ece

MONKEYS and apes have a sense of morality and the rudimentary ability to tell right from wrong, according to new research.

In a series of studies scientists have found that monkeys and apes can make judgments about fairness, offer altruistic help and empathise when a fellow animal is ill or in difficulties. They even appear to have consciences and the ability to remember obligations.

The research implies that morality is not a uniquely human quality and suggests it arose through evolution. That could mean the strength of our consciences is partly determined by our genes.

Such findings are likely to antagonise fundamentalist religious groups. Some believe the ability to form moral judgments is a God-given quality that sets humans apart.

The scientists say, however, that the evidence is clear. “I am not arguing that non-human primates are moral beings but there is enough evidence for the following of social rules to agree that some of the stepping stones towards human morality can be found in other animals,” said Frans de Waal, professor of psychology at Emory University in Georgia in the United States.

In papers at the annual meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS) this weekend de Waal described experiments on monkeys and apes to see if they understood the idea of fairness.

The animals were asked to perform a set of simple tasks and then rewarded with food or affection. The rewards were varied, seemingly at random. De Waal found the animals had an acute sense of fairness and objected strongly when others were rewarded more than themselves for the same task, often sulking and refusing to take part any further.

Another study looked at altruism in chimps - and found they were often willing to help others even when there was no obvious reward. “Chimpanzees spontaneously help both humans and each other in carefully controlled tests,” said de Waal.

Other researchers, said de Waal, have found the same qualities in capuchin monkeys, which also show “spontaneous prosocial tendencies”, meaning they are keen to share food and other gifts with other monkeys, for the pleasure of giving.

“Everything else being equal, they prefer to reward a companion together with themselves rather than just themselves,” he said. “The research suggests that giving is self-rewarding for monkeys.”

Related research found primates can remember individuals who have done them a favour and will make an effort to repay them.

De Waal, who has written a book called Primates and Philosophers, said morality appeared to have evolved in the same way as organs such as the eye and the heart, through natural selection.

The debate over whether animals can tell right from wrong and make moral choices dates back to Charles Darwin, originator of the theory of evolution.

He suggested that when sexual reproduction first evolved it forced animals to develop codes of behaviour that became built into their genes. In humans these instincts developed into a sense of right and wrong. This fitted with his view that humans were derived from animals - a view fiercely opposed by the church at the time.

The big question now is why, alone among the primates, humans have developed morality to such a high level. It implies that humans were once subjected to some kind of powerful evolutionary pressure to develop a conscience.

Some researchers believe we could owe our consciences to climate change and, in particular, to a period of intense global warming between 50,000 and 800,000 years ago. The proto-humans living in the forests had to adapt to living on hostile open plains, where they would have been easy prey for formidable predators such as big cats.

This would have forced them to devise rules for hunting in groups and sharing food.

Christopher Boehm, director of the Jane Goodall Research Center, part of the University of Southern California’s anthropology department, believes such humans devised codes to stop bigger, stronger males hogging all the food.

“To ensure fair meat distribution, hunting bands had to gang up physically against alpha males,” he said. This theory has been borne out by studies of contemporary hunter-gatherer tribes.

In research released at the AAAS he argued that under such a system those who broke the rules would have been killed, their “amoral” genes lost to posterity. By contrast, those who abided by the rules would have had many more children.

Other studies have confirmed that the strength of a person's conscience depends partly on their genes. Several researchers have shown, for example, that the children of habitual criminals will often become criminals too - even when they have had no contact with their biological parents.
 
May 9, 2002
37,066
16,282
113
#4
In all honesty, no surprises here. They are our closest relatives in the animal kingdom and are extremely intelligent.

Is it also true that dolphins, dogs, and other forms of mammals have a sense of morality? I am too lazy to Google it, but I thought i saw something on Nat Geo one evening about it.
 
Nov 10, 2006
2,124
2
0
47
#6
This chimps morality is lacking

Man Loses Face And Testicle In Ape Attack At California Sanctuary
Moe the Chimpanzee was taken from a suburban Los Angeles home where he lived with St. James and LaDonna Davis in 1999 after Moe bit off part of a part of a woman’s finger while she was visiting them.
Ok, got that part?
Well St. James and LaDonna Davis recently visited the cage in the animal sanctuary where Moe is now living to celebrate his birthday and bring him a cake.
Not that Moe could give a damn about a cake, he is just an ape, but the Davises brought him one nonetheless.
Well while they were standing in front of Moe’s cage with the birthday cake, Buddy and Ollie, two of four chimps in the adjoining cage to Moe’s somehow escaped and immediately started attacking St. James and LaDonna.
Even the two ape babes that were in the cage with Buddy and Ollie, Susie and Bones managed to escape.
Amazingly LaDonna was only bitten on the hand, consequently while trying to protect her husband St. James, who by far got the worst of the attack, which resulted in having his balls literally ripped off as well as suffering massive wounds to his face, body and limbs.
The apes actually chewed off his face.
Buddy and Ollie were shot dead at the scene but not before the damage had been done.
The true tragedy of this story is that Moe didn’t lift a finger to help St. James or LaDonna.
He just sat back and watched.
To give you an idea of how bad St. James got beat down, St. James is going to have to undergo extensive surgery to reattach his nose to his face.
There is no word yet if they are going to be able to reattach his testicles or his foot.
Yes, they also bit off this poor guy’s foot.
When the son-n-law of the sanctuary’s owner finally arrived with his pistol and shot Buddy, Ollie deduced that he was probably next but instead of just running away he
grabbed St. James, who at this time is nearly bleeding to death and literally dragged him down the road before he too was shot dead.
Maybe monkey behavioral specialists can shed some light as to why LaDonna was able to walk away with only a bite to her hand while her husband got his dick, his foot and his nose completely torn off and then to add insult to injury dragged down the road by his good foot.
So what do the experts say?
Apparently if you think Chimpanzees are these cute little apes that are a must for any loving home, think again before you put one underneath the Christmas tree.
Ape experts say that they are naturally aggressors and are known to kill rival chimps from neighboring groups, they hunt other primates and are obviously not above opening a can of “you don’t want none of this” on humans too if they forget to bring enough cake for everybody.
"Male chimps are intensely territorial. They defend their territory against any perceived threat," said Craig Stanford, a professor at the University of Southern California who studies primate behavior. "Chimps can be violent at times just as humans can be."
"Chimps can be as violent as humans can be?" Was that a little subliminal jab at the anti-evolution folks in Kansas professor Stanford?
Apparently Moe’s behavior can be at least partially explained as well.
Evidently his mother was killed by a poacher in Africa decades ago and it’s possible there is still some deep seated resentment there that needs to be addressed.
You know you always find people that are those bleeding hearts that want to take on problems like Moe and make things all better.
Well it looks like you can’t make Moe better. You just need to leave Moe alone.
Even after he bit the woman’s finger off the Davises apparently tried to launch a legal battle in the courts to bring Moe back from the sanctuary and back into their home.
That’s one reason why we have laws. To protect people from themselves apparently.
It’s not exactly clear how they feel about apes now in light of this incident. I guess we can always ask St. James after they reattach his nose, his testicles and his foot.
 
Dec 25, 2003
12,356
218
0
69
#9
My friend had a pet chimp.

Nasty fuckers.

Lil dude would sit and stare at any female in the room and masturbate furiously, with a look on his face resembling a 65 year old gay white man in leathers looking at internet pornography. And he'd hiss, spit, and throw shit at you.

As to the intelligence/social nature of monkeys, I actually saw some interesting shit on TV.

There are bands of monkeys from different species who run together and have learned each others language.

There are other monkeys who roll rocks down cliffs at leopards, spend hella time breaking nuts open with rocks, etc. There are monkeys who eat charcoal from human fires to calm their indigestion.

Monkeys have the ability to 'imagine' and to conceptualize situations that may or not happen.

http://www.pbs.org/wnet/nature/episodes/clever-monkeys/introduction/3946/

If you happen to catch that episode, watch it.
 
Jun 3, 2006
11,502
51
0
44
#10
^^lol..i remember watchin a doc about these types of little monkeys/chimps whatever, but sex was a big ass part of their culture, they would be chasin sumthin, stop fuck for 10 seconds and the take off, the mom monkeys would fuck the kids and all kinds of incest shit..they would communicate by fuckin.....it was weird ass fuck, but kinda fascinating
 
May 15, 2002
2,964
8
0
#11
^^bonobos

And it's relatively common for a loner monkey to join another group and learn their calls, eat their diet, etc.

Adaptability is the name of the game for primates.
 
Jun 3, 2006
11,502
51
0
44
#12
yep!...right on....cause i remember after watchin it i told my nephew he looked like a bonobo monkey for about a week..lol
 
May 15, 2002
2,964
8
0
#13
Chimpanzee social intelligence: selfishness, altruism, and the mother–infant bond

Abstract

To better understand the human mind from an evolutionary perspective, a great deal of research has focused on the closest living relative of humans, the chimpanzee, using various approaches, including studies of social intelligence. Here, I review recent research related to several aspects of social intelligence, including deception, understanding of perception and intention, social learning, trading, cooperation, and regard for others. Many studies have demonstrated that chimpanzees are proficient in using their social intelligence for selfish motives to benefit from their interactions with others. In contrast, it is not yet clear whether chimpanzees engage in prosocial behaviors that benefit others; however, chimpanzee mother–infant interactions indicate the possibility of such behaviors. Therefore, I propose that chimpanzees possess rudimentary traits of human mental competence not only in terms of theory of mind in a broader sense but also in terms of prosociality involving regard for others. Mother–infant interactions appear to be particularly important to understanding the manifestation of social intelligence from an evolutionary perspective.

Keywords Chimpanzees - Mother–infant relationship - Social intelligence - Theory of mind

Introduction

Three decades have passed since Premack and Woodruff (1978) coined the phrase “theory of mind.” Developmental psychological studies of human children have since used this paradigm to document how children understand each other’s minds in terms of beliefs, desires, and knowledge (e.g., Wimmer and Perner 1983; Baron-Cohen et al. 1985). This new paradigm also created a basis for the comparative study of social cognition in nonhuman primates from an evolutionary perspective. After the concept of “Machiavellian intelligence” was introduced (Byrne and Whiten 1988), researchers began to increasingly discuss its relation to primates, in terms of the theory of mind and other types of social cognition (e.g., Whiten and Byrne 1997).

Call and Tomasello (2008) summarized recent advances in the study of chimpanzees’ understanding of the minds of other individuals. They concluded that chimpanzees understand others in terms of a perception–goal psychology but not in terms of full-fledged, human-like belief–desire psychology. In other words, chimpanzees have a theory of mind in a broad sense, yet they do not possess a more narrowly defined theory of mind that requires understanding of false beliefs. Therefore, although chimpanzees lack certain human traits, humans do not possess an entirely unique social intelligence; rather, some rudimentary traits exist in chimpanzees (Call and Tomasello 2008).

However, Premack (2007) offered a different view. According to his summary, recent results in the field of brain science point to a human singularity in brain structure that is located in areas associated with complex social cognition (theory of mind) or language; in contrast, a number of animal cognition studies appear to indicate similarities between humans and nonhuman animals. Yet, after examining eight cognitive cases (i.e., teaching, short-term memory, causal reasoning, planning, deception, transitive inference, theory of mind, and language), Premack (2007) claimed that similarities between humans and animals are small and concluded that both brain and cognitive studies highlight the disparities between the human and nonhuman animal.

Here, I reconsider aspects related to social cognition in chimpanzees with reference to recent findings. First, I briefly review results of research into social intelligence, ranging from selfish deception to altruistic behavior. Second, I outline important issues in this field along with my personal observations, emphasizing the mother–infant relationship; although several of my observations are unpublished “anecdotes,” they may help elucidate points for future theoretical and empirical research.

Deception

Deceptive behavior is one of the most conspicuous actions noted when observing nonhuman primates; in particular, observers intuitively feel “intelligence” in the deceptive interactions of primates. Byrne and Whiten (1990) compiled 253 reported observations of tactical deception in primates and identified possible examples of intentional deception, which occurred more often among great apes than among monkeys, suggesting a higher level of social intelligence in apes.

During an experimental study, Hirata and Matsuzawa (2001) observed deceptive “misleading” behaviors when chimpanzees attempted to obtain a banana hidden in an enclosure (see also Hirata 2006a). While the subordinate “witness,” who had seen where the experimenter had hidden the banana, initiated an approach to the hiding place, the dominant “witness-of-the-witness,” who had not directly seen the hiding event, began to run ahead of the witness. At the same time, the witness began to try to mislead the witness-of-the-witness by going to an area where there was no banana. When the witness-of-the-witness tried to run ahead of the witness and began to search the empty area, the witness returned to the correct location and found the banana. This study was based on the pioneering work of Menzel (1974), who observed similar deceptive episodes among captive chimpanzees. Matsuzawa (1991) conducted comparable chimpanzee research at another facility and obtained results similar to those of Menzel (1974). As discussed by Whiten and Byrne (1988), deceptive episodes tend to be anecdotal, but the above three cases indicate that the deceptive ability of chimpanzees emerges quite reliably under certain experimental conditions.

Hare et al. (2006) experimentally explored another aspect of deception in chimpanzees: deception by concealment. In their study, a human experimenter competed against chimpanzees for food. If the human saw a chimpanzee approaching a piece of food, then the human retrieved the food. In response, chimpanzees approached the food from the side that the human was not facing or from the side with an opaque barrier between the chimpanzees and the human, highlighting their ability to deceive by concealment. Moreover, chimpanzees used a tactic of indirect approach by initially distancing themselves from the food while in view of the human and then approaching the food later when behind the human or the barrier (Hare et al. 2006). Overall, these results support the view that chimpanzees know what others can and cannot see.

Understanding of perception and intention

The above examples of deception reinforce the notion that chimpanzees understand the visual perception of others. In a series of experiments with chimpanzees, Hare et al. (2000, 2001) and Bräuer et al. (2007) more explicitly tested these abilities. With several variations, the basic set-up of their experiments included a dominant and subordinate pair facing pieces of food which the dominant individual could or could not see, had been shown or had not been shown, or did or did not know about. If the subordinate individual understood the visual perspective of the dominant, he/she would approach a piece of food that the dominant could not see, had not seen, or did not know about, in order to avoid conflict over food with the dominant. The results supported the theory that subordinate individuals understand the visual perspective of dominant individuals.

In addition to understanding what others can and cannot see, researchers have investigated whether chimpanzees comprehend the goals of the actions of others (Call and Tomasello 1998; Call et al. 2004; Myowa-Yamakoshi and Matsuzawa 2000; Tomasello and Carpenter 2005). In these studies, the responses of chimpanzees were recorded after they observed an actor performing an action that did not meet the actor’s intended goals, such as unsuccessful attempts at object manipulations. The results of these studies collectively indicated that chimpanzees understand the goals or intentions of others.

Social learning and the absence of active teaching

Whether in the wild or in captivity, an individual will often benefit from the behaviors of others in a social learning context. Wild chimpanzees socially learn to use tools and learn other forms of behavior from their mothers or other group members (e.g., McGrew 1992). Numerous studies in nature and in captivity have investigated phenomena involving social learning. Whereas observations in the wild have demonstrated the existence of culture among chimpanzees (e.g., Whiten et al. 1999), studies in captivity suggest that true imitation is more difficult for chimpanzees than was initially thought (e.g., Myowa-Yamakoshi and Matsuzawa 1999). However, research in captivity has begun to reveal social features of chimpanzees, such as conformity to the group norm and traditions over generations (Hopper et al. 2007; Horner et al. 2006; Whiten et al. 2005, 2007), all of which support the theory of “culture” among wild populations in Africa.

In contrast to their excellent ability to learn socially, an intriguing fact is that chimpanzees lack active teaching. For example, in three mother–infant pairs, Hirata and Celli (2003) studied the process by which chimpanzee infants learn tool use in the presence of skilled mothers (see also Hirata 2006b). Infants repeatedly and carefully observed their mothers and other adults and mastered tool-use behavior after the spontaneous observation of successful performances by other individuals. However, there were no cases that could be interpreted as active teaching by a mother. The mothers never exhibited key active-teaching behaviors, such as guiding the infants’ hands or performing the task for their babies.

Matsuzawa et al. (2001) used the term “master apprenticeship” to characterize the chimpanzee cultural process. During this process, a chimpanzee “master,” who is skilled in a certain type of tool use, does not actively teach the chimpanzee “apprentice,” who is naïve in the use of this tool. Rather, through long-term repetitive observations of the master that are supported by high levels of tolerance by the master (e.g., allowing access to tools and to food obtained via tool use), the apprentice acquires the skill.

Trading and role-taking

An individual may also benefit through interacting with others by engaging in trading. Trading in nonhuman primates has become the focus of recent studies under the framework of the biological market (Noë 2001), in which individuals use mental capacity to exchange currency, such as grooming and support for other group members, in a social market (i.e., their group). In Kanyawara, Uganda, Duffy et al. (2007) reported a positive correlation between mating success in wild chimpanzee males and their level of support of the alpha male, indicating that the alpha male in this group selectively tolerated mating by his allies and exchanged mating tolerance for support in conflicts. In another group of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea, Hockings et al. (2007) examined sharing of cultivated plant foods among group members. In this group, sharing primarily consisted of adult males sharing with reproductively cycling females by offering exchanges of food for mating or grooming.

Another interesting phenomenon has been observed within the complex social network of wild chimpanzees at Bossou. When chimpanzees at Bossou encounter the road that cuts through their home range, they typically do not cross alone but gather at a certain spot in the bushes along the road and march in parade. Crossing the roads is risky, because they have to leave the forest and enter open areas where they encounter humans and vehicles. Hockings et al. (2006) examined the progression order of parties of chimpanzees crossing the roads. Typically, the second-ranking male scanned the road, the first-ranking male took the rearmost position, and females and young individuals occupied the more protected middle positions. Thus, the males engaged in role-taking activity in this situation. Hockings et al. (2006) concluded that dominant chimpanzees act cooperatively with a high level of flexibility to maximize group protection.

Cooperation

As indicated in the example above, chimpanzees may cooperate with each other. Boesch and Boesch (1989) suggested that chimpanzees at Taï, Côte d’Ivoire, hunt their prey cooperatively. Several chimpanzees chase the target, and when the target runs away, several other chimpanzees lie in wait to capture it. However, cooperative hunting in chimpanzees has been debated (e.g., Gilby et al. 2006).

Several experimental studies have tested cooperative ability in captive individuals, demonstrating that two chimpanzees were able to act together to pull a heavy box to obtain food (Crawford 1937) or to pull a handle to make food fall from a dispenser (Chalmeau 1994). Similar experimental studies have been recently revisited. Povinelli and O’Neill (2000) used a method identical to that of Crawford (1937) to investigate whether an individual who had mastered the skill necessary for a task would instruct a naïve partner; however, they found no such evidence. Hirata and Fuwa (2007) designed a new task in which two chimpanzees were each required to pull one end of a rope simultaneously to drag blocks supporting food into reach (see also Hirata 2007; Hirata et al. in press). The chimpanzees did not succeed during initial tests, in that they did not immediately understand the necessity for cooperation and did not adjust their behavior to work with the partner. However, in subsequent tests, the frequency of success gradually increased. Thus, the two chimpanzees learned to coordinate their own behavior with that of the partner by watching the partner and waiting if necessary. Interestingly, the chimpanzees did not use mutual eye contact or behavioral signs to achieve mutual coordination (Hirata and Fuwa 2007; but see Crawford, 1937). Melis et al. (2006a) used an experimental method that was fundamentally identical to the task described above. They demonstrated that (a) chimpanzees could recruit a partner to collaborate only when necessary, and (b) they recruited the more effective of two partners for collaboration on the basis of their past experience with each partner, concluding also that tolerance plays an important role in chimpanzee cooperative actions (Melis et al. 2006b).

Altruism and regard for others

The step beyond cooperation is altruism or regard for others. Warneken et al. (2006, 2007) and Warneken and Tomasello (2006) explored altruism in chimpanzees in a series of experiments in which chimpanzees observed a human trying to reach an object or another chimpanzee trying to open a door. Their results offered evidence for altruistic behavior, as the chimpanzees helped conspecifics to open the door and both familiar and unfamiliar humans to reach an object.

In contrast, other experimental studies have found negative evidence for altruism. Silk et al. (2005), Vonk et al. (2008), and Jensen et al. (2006) investigated whether chimpanzees would provide food to other group members. In these studies, the chimpanzees could choose whether to provide a food reward only to themselves or simultaneously to themselves and another member of the group. Results consistently indicated that chimpanzees would not offer food to the other group members. Silk et al. (2005) concluded that chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of group members.

An interim summary: predominance of selfish motives?

The above research into the social intelligence of chimpanzees indicates that they are skillful at interacting with others in various ways, but their primary motivations are selfish. For example, these experiments have clearly demonstrated selfish motives during interactions such as deception, social learning, and trading. Chimpanzees seek benefits by deceiving other individuals in competitive situations and by observing a skilled master in social learning situations, and they expect future benefits when engaging in trading. Moreover, chimpanzees solve even cooperative tasks for selfish motives, in that they work together with partners because they selfishly desire the resulting food reward. Indeed, in an experimental study, Hirata and Fuwa (2007) never observed chimpanzees helping partners to obtain food (see also Hirata et al. in press). In addition, chimpanzees lack active teaching in social learning situations, which supports the hypothesis that they are indifferent to the benefits of others. Furthermore, Vonk et al. (2008) suggested that the results of Warneken et al. (2006, 2007) can also be interpreted from a selfish perspective, as chimpanzees may help others in expectation of obtaining future rewards.

The above interpretation is in concert with the view of Tomasello et al. (2005), that apes lack shared intentionality. According to their description, shared intentionality consists of understanding the goals, intentions, and perceptions of another individual, along with a motivation to share these things in interaction with others; that is, “we” intentionality. The predominance of selfish motives described in the above paragraph may be linked to the absence of motivation to share intentionality, as proposed by Tomasello et al. (2005). Chimpanzee behaviors in many situations are caused not by “we” intentionality but by “me” desire.

However, is it really the case that chimpanzees only act cooperatively for selfish motives? Predominance of selfish motives does not necessarily mean absence of shared intentionality. If there is a situation in which the chimpanzees’ selfish motive is diminished, then they may show truly cooperative behaviors or other-regarding behaviors. The predominance of selfish motives is not entirely synonymous with the hypothesized lack of “we” intentionality. Vonk et al. (2008) briefly noted that other-regarding sentiments are not activated when food is present, because food is an object of competition. In addition to this possibility, my own experience has revealed other complications related to testing chimpanzees in experimental situations. I conduct touch-screen tasks with chimpanzees at the Hayashibara Great Ape Research Institute (Hirata, unpublished data). If a chimpanzee solves a task by touching the touch screen, he/she can receive a food reward. The touch-screen tasks are conducted in two locations: an outdoor booth in an open enclosure and an indoor experimental booth. Interestingly, the motivations of chimpanzees differ greatly between these two locations. When a task is performed in the outdoor booth, it is completed very easily; a 1–2-g piece of apple is a sufficient reward for a successful trial, and chimpanzees repeat the task for >200 trials (sometimes >2000 trials) with no problems. In contrast, when chimpanzees are taken individually to the indoor experimental booth, they all become reluctant to perform the same task. Even if I offer one-eighth of an apple (approximately 30 g) for a successful trial, chimpanzees often exhibit a gesture of refusal. Thus, even if the basic structure of the task remains the same (i.e., a certain behavior results in a food reward), differences in the environment that seemingly have no direct relationship to the task itself greatly alter the attitude of the chimpanzees (see also Hirata 2006a). Thus, it is incorrect to naïvely presume that a food reward can induce any type of behaviors that a chimpanzee has the potential to perform in any circumstances. The failure to create a situation in which chimpanzees will behave a certain way does not necessarily mean that the animals lack an ability to behave in that manner. As de Waal (in press) pointed out, absence of evidence does not mean evidence of absence.

Regard for others and the mother–infant relationship

In this second part of the paper I offer my observations of chimpanzees, accompanied by illustrative anecdotes, to rethink the possibility of other-regarding behaviors that cannot be interpreted solely as products of selfish motives.

Food and object sharing

Regarding the conclusion of Vonk et al. (2008) that chimpanzees do not actively offer food, I present a case of food sharing. Vonk et al. (2008) referred to a claim that food exchange in chimpanzees is better described as “tolerated theft” than voluntary sharing, or that it can be explained by calculated self-interest to gain benefits in later interactions. However, these explanations appear to be unsatisfactory in some cases of food sharing, the most frequent type of which occurs between mothers and infants. Ueno and Matsuzawa (2004) quantitatively analyzed food sharing in three mother–infant pairs of captive chimpanzees and determined that mothers generally do not offer preferable parts of food material to their infants. Therefore, in many cases, food sharing between the chimpanzee mother and infant does not involve the mother offering preferred foods to the infant. However, other cases exist in which a mother actively offered a preferred food item to the infant following the infant’s begging (see Fig. 1). The active offering of food by the mother appears to imply something beyond just “tolerated theft.” In addition, mothers do not expect anything in return from the young infant; thus, it is difficult to interpret such cases as calculated self-interest.

Hirata and Celli (2003) observed offerings of a tool by mothers to their infants during the process by which infants learn tool use to fish for honey. In such cases, the tools offered by adults to infants almost never had honey on them; mothers only offered a tool to infants after they had already licked the honey from it or before they had used the tool. Therefore, mothers generally did not offer a reward to the infant. However, the mothers sometimes did not merely allow the infants to take the tool from their hands, but they actively offered tools to infants (see Fig. 2). The tolerated theft hypothesis seems insufficient to explain the active behavior of the mothers, and these behaviors of the mothers cannot be explained as calculated self-interest, as in the case of food sharing between mothers and infants.

Instead, active food or object sharing may constitute a step toward human-like regard for others. As these cases most often occur between the mother and infant, the mother–infant bond may have provided a rudimentary basis for the evolution of other-regard or altruism.

Teaching by inhibition

According to Premack (2007), teaching in humans is completely different from teaching in nonhuman animals. Teaching in humans is a domain-general competence based on theory of mind, whereas teaching in animals, such as a cat injuring mice and bringing them to her kittens, is an adaptation targeted to a single goal. Chimpanzees generally do not actively teach others. For example, Hirata and Celli (2003) conducted detailed observations of mothers and infants during situations in which infants learn tool use, and there were no instances of active teaching.
While teaching by encouragement (i.e., encouraging another individual to do something) has not been observed in chimpanzees, except for the observations by Boesch (1991), teaching by inhibition (i.e., preventing another individual from doing something) has been reported on several occasions. For example, when wild chimpanzee infants approach a plant that is not included in the diet of the population, the mothers pull the infants away from the plant (Hiraiwa-Hasegawa 1990), and this action can be interpreted as an example of teaching by inhibition. I have observed several cases that can be classified as teaching by inhibition in a mother–infant pair at the Hayashibara Great Ape Research Institute. For experimental purposes, several nut-cracking sites consisting of an anvil and hammer stones have been created in an open enclosure. The hammer stones are fixed to the ground by a stainless steel chain to prevent the chimpanzees from moving or throwing them away. When an infant female chimpanzee born to this group began to explore the enclosure by herself, she tried to play with the chain that was connected to the stones. As the infant was still clumsy in her manual actions, the infant’s hand or fingers could possibly have become tangled in the chain, causing damage. When the mother witnessed her infant playing with the chain, she approached her and smoothly removed the chain from the infant (Fig. 3). Similar behaviors were observed several times until the infant had matured. These examples indicate that the chimpanzee mother tried to stop a certain behavior of the infant, who benefited by being kept away from an unknown danger.

Premack (2007) stated that human teaching consists of three distinct actions involving observation, judgment, and modification. The teaching by inhibition performed by the chimpanzee mother illustrates her ability to observe and judge the situation experienced by the infant and appropriately modify its behavior.

Communication

Chimpanzee mothers and infants communicate using vocalizations and gestures. Hirata (2008) observed numerous interactions of mother–infant pairs in captivity. When infants were immature, mothers sometimes helped the infants to move. When the mothers traveled longer distances that might have been difficult for the infant to travel alone, the mother communicated with the infant, and they traveled together with the mother carrying the infant. For example, prior to travel, one mother stretched out her hand toward her offspring who was some distance away from her (Fig. 4). The infant then approached its mother to take her hand. The mother cradled the infant and moved from one location to another while carrying her offspring. In one-third to one-half of cases, such contact between the mother and infant was established by mutual communication, and the longer the mother traveled, the more often she established contact with the infant to carry it. Hirata (2008) suggested that mothers may determine the goal of travel in advance and carry the infant if necessary, communicating with the infant in advance using several types of behaviors.

Several recent studies have focused on gestural communication in chimpanzees and have demonstrated that wild chimpanzees engage in referential communication (Pika and Mitani 2006). Pollick and de Waal (2007) demonstrated that the flexibility of gestural communication is greater compared to facial/vocal signals, which suggests that gestural communication may be the root of human language. The mother–infant relationship is the first social relationship that an infant experiences, and the ability to produce and understand gestural communication can be observed early in infancy. In this sense, the mother–infant relationship may have fostered the ability of gestural communication in primate evolution.

Concluding remarks

Advanced social intelligence in chimpanzees enables them to engage in deceptive interactions, perspective-taking, social learning, trading, and cooperative actions. Many studies have revealed that chimpanzees are proficient in using their social intelligence for selfish motives. In contrast, it is not yet clear whether these primates have tendencies to engage in prosocial behaviors to benefit others, and both positive and negative experimental evidences have recently been reported. However, episodes of chimpanzee mother–infant interactions indicate the possibility of prosocial behavior. Therefore, unlike the views of Premack (2007) and Vonk et al. (2008), I propose that chimpanzees possess rudimentary traits of human mental competence, not only in terms of theory of mind in a broader sense, but also in terms of prosociality involving regard for others.

De Waal (2008) argued that empathy plays a crucial role in altruistic behaviors. Three levels of empathy are postulated in his arguments: emotional contagion as the lowest level, sympathetic concern as the next evolutionary step, and empathic perspective-taking as the highest level. He describes the case of a mother ape who helps a whimpering juvenile move from one tree to the next as evidence of empathic perspective-taking, because the mother assesses the juvenile’s goal and the specific reason for the youngster’s distress. The case of mother chimpanzees communicating with their infants when they travel, as described in the earlier section, as well as the case of mother–infant food or object-sharing fit within the idea of empathic perspective-taking. A point of difference is that, in contrast to the example of a mother’s helping her whimpering offspring in de Waal’s (2008) paper, infant chimpanzees described in the above section do not show signs of distress. Therefore, in the first place, it is clear that the mothers’ behaviors in the latter cases cannot be ascribed to emotional contagion or sympathetic concern. Rather, these cases indicate a possibility that chimpanzee mothers understand the situation of their infants and attempt to meet their needs even without emotional signals from the infants; this form of response may constitute a higher grade of empathetic perspective-taking.

Mother–infant relationships have experienced numerous changes during mammalian, primate, and hominoid evolution, from merely provisioning milk to long-term care by embracing (Matsuzawa 2006). Chimpanzee mother–infant interactions exhibit features common to humans, such as face-to-face communication (Tomonaga et al. 2004). The mother–infant relationship has become increasingly intense during the course of evolution and represents the relationship in which newborn infants first experience sociality. Therefore, it is quite logical to deduce that mother–infant interactions are key to understanding the manifestation of social intelligence from an evolutionary perspective.

Several studies have examined the cognitive ability of chimpanzee infants reared by humans, particularly in ape-language projects (e.g., Gardner and Gardner 1969). In addition, some research has investigated social intelligence between unrelated adults (e.g. Hirata and Matsuzawa 2001; Hirata and Fuwa 2007); however, little information is available regarding the sociocognitive abilities underlying interactions between chimpanzee infants and their biological mothers (but see the “Ai project” since 2000; Matsuzawa 2003). Future studies are required to better understand both the continuities and discontinuities in cognitive traits between human and nonhuman animals and to reveal causes of the evolution of uniquely human cognitive faculties, such as complex, domain-general, and embedded understanding of the minds of others.
From Primates 50(1): 3-11
 
May 9, 2002
37,066
16,282
113
#14
^^lol..i remember watchin a doc about these types of little monkeys/chimps whatever, but sex was a big ass part of their culture, they would be chasin sumthin, stop fuck for 10 seconds and the take off, the mom monkeys would fuck the kids and all kinds of incest shit..they would communicate by fuckin.....it was weird ass fuck, but kinda fascinating
From Wiki:

Sexual social behavior

Sexual intercourse plays a major role in bonobo society, being used as a greeting, a means of conflict resolution, and post-conflict reconciliation. With the exception of a pair of Cohan gorillas observed doing so,[18] Bonobos were thought to be the only non-human apes to have been observed engaging in all of the following sexual activities: face-to-face genital sex, tongue kissing, and oral sex.[19] In scientific literature, the female-female sexual behavior often is referred to as GG rubbing,genital-genital rubbing, or "scissoring".

Sexual activity happens within the immediate family as well as outside it.[20] Bonobos never form permanent relationships with individual partners. They also do not seem to discriminate in their sexual behavior by gender or age, with the possible exception of abstaining from sexual intercourse between mothers and their adult sons; some observers believe these pairings are taboo. When Bonobos come upon a new food source or feeding ground, the increased excitement will usually lead to communal sexual activity, presumably decreasing tension and allowing for peaceful feeding.[21]

Bonobo males frequently engage in various forms of male-male genital sexual behavior (frot).[22][23] In one form, two males hang from a tree limb face-to-face while "penis fencing".[24][25] Frot also may occur when two males rub their penises together while in face to face position. A special form of frot called "rump rubbing" occurs to express reconciliation between two males after a conflict, when they stand back-to-back and rub their scrotal sacs together.

Bonobo females also engage in female-female genital sexual behavior, (tribadism), to bond socially with each other, thus forming a female nucleus of Bonobo society. The bonding among females allows them to dominate Bonobo society - although male Bonobos are individually stronger, they cannot stand alone against a united group of females.[25] Adolescent females often leave their native community to join another community. Sexual bonding with other females establishes the new females as members of the group. This migration mixes the Bonobo gene pools, providing genetic diversity.

Bonobo reproductive rates are not any higher than that of the Common Chimpanzee. Female Bonobos carry and nurse their young for five years and can give birth every five to six years. Compared to Common Chimpanzees, Bonobo females resume the genital swelling cycle much sooner after giving birth, allowing them to rejoin the sexual activities of their society. Also, Bonobo females who are sterile or too young to reproduce still engage in sexual activity.

Craig Stanford, an American primatologist, has challenged the claim that Bonobos are more sexually active than Common Chimpanzees. Stanford compared existing data on Common Chimpanzees and Bonobos in the natural habitat and found that female Common Chimpanzees copulated at least as often as female Bonobos, while he recorded that male chimpanzees copulated more than male Bonobos.[26] His comparison excluded same-sex sexual contacts, however, which are very common in Bonobos. De Waal's book on Bonobos includes interviews with field workers and relies on the studies by Takayoshi Kano, the only scientist to have worked for two decades with wild Bonobos.[27] New studies in Africa by Gottfried Hohmann, suggest occasional violence, but the fact remains that there are thus far no documented cases of lethal aggression among Bonobos, in sharp contrast to the evidence for Common Chimpanzees.[28]

These chimps are SUPER freaks!
 
May 9, 2002
37,066
16,282
113
#16
Here is a funny story...

http://www.nydailynews.com/news/200...auled_by_outofcontrol_pet_ape_tra.html?page=1

Woman mauled by out-of-control pet ape Travis in critical condition after surgery at Conn. hospital

BY Edgar Sandoval, Bill Hutchinson and Helen Kennedy
DAILY NEWS STAFF WRITERS

Tuesday, February 17th 2009, 3:02 PM


The Connecticut woman brutally mauled by a gigantic pet ape remained in critical condition Tuesday, officials said.

Charla Nash, 55, suffered massive injuries to her face and hands after her friend's 200-pound chimp, Travis, suddenly pounced on her Monday.

Nash had surgery last night at Stamford Hospital and was doing "okay," her sister-in-law, Kate Nash said.

Nash suffered what Stamford Mayor Daniel Malloy described as "life-changing, if not life-threatening injuries" after Travis got his hands on house keys and escaped from his home.

Travis attacked Nash as soon as she got out of her car to try to help his owner, Sandy Herold, get the ape back in his cage, said Stamford, Conn., police Capt. Rich Conklin.

Herold, stabbed Travis with a butcher knife and struck him with a shovel in a bid to end his rampage, police said.

But the normally docile chimp, who starred in a TV commercial for Old Navy cargo shorts and enjoyed human activities like drinking wine and surfing the Internet, could not be stopped.

When cops drove up around 4 p.m., the burly ape tore off a cruiser's side mirror and opened the door.

The officers had "nowhere to retreat," Conklin said, and shot him several times.

Bleeding from stab wounds and gunshots, 15-year-old Travis staggered down the driveway and into Herold's house, where he collapsed and died in a zoolike cage the size of a room.

Conklin said it wasn't clear what set the chimp off, but theorized Travis' bout with Lyme disease - which can cause panic attacks, paranoia, personality changes and mood swings in people - could be connected. Travis was taking medication for the disease, Conklin said.

"These actions have not been seen in the chimpanzee before. This animal had been raised as a member of the family," Conklin said.

Herold, 70, had given the pyscho simian tea with Xanax to calm him down just before Nash arrived, cops said, and called 911 as the chimp mauled her friend.

A 911 dispatcher could hear the animal screaming in the background as he ripped into the victim.

Herold told cops that Travis may not have recognized Charla Nash because she wore her normally long, flowing hair up Monday.

Herold and her husband, Jerome, who treated Travis almost like a child after the death of their daughter more than a decade ago, were distraught.

Conklin said the couple had owned Travis for close to 14 years

Herold's friend Lynn Mecca said Travis had known Charla Nash for years.

"I don't know why he would do that," she said.

But Mecca's ex-husband Don Mecca said the ape had a mean streak and often was aggressive. He said he had warned Nash to be careful around Travis. "I told her, 'Charla, don't get close to that monkey when he is not in that cage,'" Mecca said.

Nash's brother Steve said his sister was aware Travis could be moody.

Travis was known to the town cops because they worked with the Herolds' towing company, Desire Me Towing. He would ride in the trucks, waving.

When the Herolds' daughter was killed in a car crash, the ape appeared to mourn, holding her photo sadly, they told the paper.

In October 2003, the chimp made headlines when he jumped out of his owners' SUV and commandeered a major intersection, holding cops at bay for two hours.

Travis had been in the vehicle when it stopped at a light, and someone in the next vehicle threw something at him, hitting him through an open window.

He unbuckled his seat belt and jumped out of the vehicle, wanting to play.

Cops arrived in a dozen cruisers but could not corral Travis, who was capering in the street, occasionally rolling on his back and charging at officers.

Officers, who had no tranquilizer gun, tried using cookies, macadamia nuts and ice cream to lure Travis into a cruiser. Nothing worked until he tired and got back into the Herolds' SUV.

No charges were filed in that case. It is not illegal to own an exotic pet in Connecticut.

"That was more mischievous than vicious," Conklin said. "It became something of a legend."
Travis was also something of a celebrity in his younger days.

He co-starred with Morgan Fairchild in an Old Navy ad, filmed a TV pilot and appeared on "The Maury Povich Show."

"Our closest relatives, we would like to think they can be domesticated," Malloy said Monday night.

"Unfortunately, this is not always the case."